Don’t stress the could haves. If it should have, it would have. — Anonymous
If there’s one thing humans are good at, it’s imagining how things could have gone differently. From those perfect shower-time comebacks to those infinite routes you could have taken to avoid a crash, we people often engage in what’s known as counterfactual thinking.
And although these counterfactuals are firmly rooted in our imagination, they can have surprising effects on our perception of truth. You know, that thing supposed to based entirely on unchanging, literal facts.
OUR OH SO TRICKY MIND
Imagine you’re going to take a commercial flight to go see some friends in another state. As you leave for the airport, you hit unexpected traffic on the freeway. When you finally get there, you rush to security, make it through relatively quickly, and get to your gate only to find you are 5 minutes too late.
Now, imagine the same scenario, but instead of 5 minutes late, you were 30 minutes late. Which of those two realities feels worse?
To most people, being 5 (vs. 30) minutes late feels way worse. But in reality, you’re unable to take your flight in both cases, and so both scenarios should feel equally bad. Why don’t they?
Researchers call it the emotional amplification effect: the easier it is to imagine how things could have gone differently, the quicker and stronger those emotions ramp up.
For example, when you’re 5 minutes late, you think about that yellow light you could have ran or that suitcase you could have packed faster. But when you’re 30 minutes late, you don’t really consider all those things. So even though you wasted a plane ticket in both scenarios, the ease with which those counterfactuals come to mind really influence how painful that loss feels.
LIES, LIES, AND MORE LIES
At its core, a counterfactual is a “lie” we tell ourselves of how things could have gone. And although it makes sense these can affect our emotions, surprisingly, they can affect our behaviors, too.
Researchers ran a fun study where they had participants roll a die for money. The higher the number rolled, the more the participant won. Importantly, the participants did their rolling in secret (i.e., under a cup) and were later asked to personally report what number had come up.
Now, for half the participants, they rolled the dice once and reported their score. However, for the other half of participants, they rolled the dice two more times after the first roll, but were only asked to report the first roll.
Now, for those who only rolled once, 19% reported that they rolled a 6 (i.e., the highest paying number). And statically, this is pretty accurate: 17% of the time, a 6 should have come up. But for those who took those two extra rolls–those who saw how things could have gone differently–34% of them reported rolling a 6.
That is, when we see or imagine how events could have been otherwise, we’re more comfortable with accepting falsehoods as truth. Especially when these falsehoods align with our desires.
IT DOESN’T STOP THERE
There are few motivational biases as strong as our political affiliations. For example, consider the following two statements related to the 2016 US Presidential election:
(1) It’s a proven fact that Donald Trump won the electoral vote but lost the popular vote to Hillary Clinton. However, if Republicans had not passed such strict voting laws, then Clinton would have won the electoral vote.
(2) It’s a proven fact that Hillary Clinton won the popular vote but lost the electoral election to Donald Trump. However, if Trump had tried to win the popular vote, then he would have won it.
With both statements, the first half is true and the second half is a counterfactual–how things could have gone differently. Researchers then compared how these counterfactuals influenced people’s perceptions of lying.
That is, when Democrats read the first statement along with the counterfactual (vs. just the true part at the beginning), they were more comfortable with people saying Clinton technically won the electoral vote (even though that’s an explicit lie). At the same time, when Republicans read the second statement along with the counterfactual (vs. just the true part), they, too, were more comfortable with people saying Trump technically won the popular vote (this, too, being an explicit lie).
In other words, we don’t just use counterfactuals to misrepresent the truth in our minds, but also to misrepresent the truth in the world. Now, just think how you would go about your life if you hadn’t read today’s post? 😉
Counterfactually,
jdt
Psych•o•philosophy to Ponder: Although we’ve talked about ways that counterfactuals can lead us astray (e.g., ramping up our emotions, making dishonesty more permissible) can you think of ways that counterfactuals could be used for “good?” For example, have you ever had an instance where you thought, “Phew! That could have been so much worse…” How can using a counterfactual like this promote positive outcomes? But also, how, too, can it produce negative ones?
Effron, D. A. (2018). It Could Have Been True: How Counterfactual Thoughts Reduce Condemnation of Falsehoods and Increase Political Polarization. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(5), 729-745.
Miller, D. T., & McFarland, C. (1986). Counterfactual thinking and victim compensation: A test of norm theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12(4), 513-519.
Shalvi, S., Dana, J., Handgraaf, M. J., & De Dreu, C. K. (2011). Justified ethicality: Observing desired counterfactuals modifies ethical perceptions and behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115(2), 181-190.
Anonymous
Jake, take a look if you will:https://aeon.co/essays/are-humans-really-blind-to-the-gorilla-on-the-basketball-court
Grist for thy mill? And… check out the linked David Deutsch essay too if your busy life allows.
And… might you not find Aeon, a worthy forum for yourself?
jdt
Wow, that was a really fascinating article and interesting perspective on such a classic study; so often our understanding of social/behavioral phenomena comes down to the perspective or question being asked. Thank you for sharing. And you know, I wasn’t very familiar with that author or that website. After checking out a few other articles there, though, I must say it is a forum that captures my curiosities. Again, thank you for sharing it! And also, I’d never heard of that expression (“girst for thy mind”). I learned multiple things with your comment today, thank you 🙂